Arquivo da tag: technology

More technology, less interaction ~ Philip Schmohl

The Great Lie

Used to be when you wanted to get your point across you actually got in someone’s face – no more. The disassociative nature of technology is separating and fragmenting our society rather than bringing it together. It is called ‘social networking’, but is it really?! Sure, there is connection in some way, but from the lonely dark rooms of our homes or worse yet, crowded rooms where we are cubicled off from each other. The great lie being propagated as the panacia to a one world, global society was that technology was going to cure our ills by bringing ideas and people together. Have you experienced a sense of joining with the world lately, or do you feel more closed off than ever before?

Take the advent of television: what began to happen? Families sat in a passive state as entertainment came at us one way only, no interacting with it. Thus we see whole families sitting idle, deadened expressions, faces awash in flickering cathode rays. The family card nights, board games, charades and alike falling by the wayside. T.v. trays took the place of dinner tables, as our society fell under the spell of this new broadcast medium.  …and that was only the beginning. Continue lendo

Facebook Profiles Reflect Actual Personality, Not Self-Idealization

Global background with glowing red rings.

Mitja D. Back, Department of Psychology, Johannes Gutenberg-University, Mainz, 55099, Mainz, Germany E-mail: back@uni-mainz.de

More than 700 million people worldwide now have profiles on on-line social networking sites (OSNs), such as MySpace and Facebook (ComScore, 2008); OSNs have become integrated into the milieu of modern-day social interactions and are widely used as a primary medium for communication and networking (boyd & Ellison, 2007; Valkenburg & Peter, 2009). Despite the increasing integration of OSN activity into everyday life, however, there has been no research on the most fundamental question about OSN profiles: Do they convey accurate impressions of profile owners?

A widely held assumption, supported by content analyses, suggests that OSN profiles are used to create and communicate idealized selves (Manago, Graham, Greenfield, & Salimkhan, 2008). According to this idealized virtual-identity hypothesis, profile owners display idealized characteristics that do not reflect their actual personalities. Thus, personality impressions based on OSN profiles should reflect profile owners’ ideal-self views rather than what the owners are actually like.

A contrasting view holds that OSNs may constitute an extended social context in which to express one’s actual personality characteristics, thus fostering accurate interpersonal perceptions. OSNs integrate various sources of personal information that mirror those found in personal environments, private thoughts, facial images, and social behavior, all of which are known to contain valid information about personality (Ambady & Skowronski, 2008; Funder, 1999; Hall & Bernieri, 2001; Kenny, 1994; Vazire & Gosling, 2004). Moreover, creating idealized identities should be hard to accomplish because (a) OSN profiles include information about one’s reputation that is difficult to control (e.g., wall posts) and (b) friends provide accountability and subtle feedback on one’s profile. Accordingly, the extended real-life hypothesis predicts that people use OSNs to communicate their real personality. If this supposition is true, lay observers should be able to accurately infer the personality characteristics of OSN profile owners. In the present study, we tested the two competing hypotheses.

Method – Participants

Participants were 236 OSN users (ages 17–22 years) from the most popular OSNs in the United States (Facebook; N = 133, 52 male, 81 female) and Germany (StudiVZ, SchuelerVZ; N = 103, 17 male, 86 female). In the United States, participants were recruited from the University of Texas campus, where flyers and candy were used to find volunteers for a laboratory-based study of personality judgment. Participants were compensated with a combination of money and course credit. In Germany, participants were recruited through advertisements for an on-line study on personality measurement. As compensation, they received individual feedback on their personality scores.

To ensure that participants did not alter their OSN profiles, we saved their profiles before the subject of OSNs was raised. Scores on all measures were normally distributed.

Measures – Accuracy criteria

Accuracy criteria (i.e., indices of what profile owners were actually like) were created by aggregating across multiple personality reports, each of which measured the Big Five personality dimensions (John, Naumann, & Soto, 2008). In the U.S. sample, profile owners’ self-reports and reports from four well-acquainted friends were obtained using the Ten Item Personality Inventory (TIPI; Gosling, Rentfrow, & Swann, 2003). In the German sample, self-reports on the short form of the Big Five Inventory (BFI-10; Rammstedt & John, 2007) and the NEO Five-Factor Inventory (Costa & McCrae, 1992) were combined.

Ideal-self ratings

We measured ideal-self perceptions by rephrasing the TIPI and the BFI-10 rating instructions: Participants were asked to “describe yourself as you ideally would like to be.”

Observer ratings

Observer ratings (how profile owners were perceived) were obtained from 9 (U.S. sample) and 10 (German sample) undergraduate research assistants, who perused each OSN profile without time restrictions and then rated their impressions of the profile owners using an observer-report form of the TIPI (U.S. sample) or BFI-10 (German sample). Each observer rated only profiles of participants from his or her own country. Observer agreement (consensus) was calculated within each sample using intraclass correlations (ICCs) for both single, ICC(2,1), and aggregate, ICC(2,k), ratings. Consensus was then averaged across samples using Fisher’s r-to-z transformation (see Table 1, column 1).

Analyses

In each sample, we determined accuracy by correlating the aggregated observer ratings with the accuracy criterion. To gauge the effect of self-idealization, we computed partial correlations between profile owners’ ideal-self ratings and aggregated observer ratings, controlling for the accuracy criterion; this procedure removed the reality component from ideal-self ratings to leave a pure measure of self-idealization.1 To determine whether results were consistent across samples, we computed a dummy-coded variable, “U.S. versus German sample,” and ran general linear models, including all interactive effects. No significant interactions emerged. Thus, to obtain the most robust estimates of the effect sizes, we first z-standardized all data within each sample, then combined the samples, and then ran the analyses again. To provide an estimate of accuracy and self-idealization effects for a single observer (not inflated by aggregation), we also calculated the effects separately for each observer and then averaged across observers using Fisher’s r-to-z transformation (Hall & Bernieri, 2001). Significance testing was done by means of one-sample t tests, using observer as the unit of analysis.

Results and Discussion

Our results were consistent with the extended real-life hypothesis and contrary to the idealized virtual-identity hypothesis. Observer accuracy was found, but there was no evidence of self-idealization (see Table 1), and ideal-self ratings did not predict observer impressions above and beyond actual personality. In contrast, even when controlling for ideal-self ratings, the effect of actual personality on OSN impressions remained significant for nearly all analyses. Accuracy was strongest for extraversion (paralleling results from face-to-face encounters) and openness (similar to research on personal environments). Accuracy was lowest for neuroticism, which is consistent with previous research showing that neuroticism is difficult to detect in all zero-acquaintance contexts (Funder, 1999; Kenny, 1994). These results suggest that people are not using their OSN profiles to promote an idealized virtual identity. Instead, OSNs might be an efficient medium for expressing and communicating real personality, which may help explain their popularity.

Our findings represent a first look at the accuracy of people’s self-portrayals on OSNs. To clarify the processes and moderating factors involved, future research should investigate (a) older users and other OSNs, (b) other personality traits, (c) other forms of impression management, (d) the role of specific profile components (e.g., photos, preferences), and (e) individual differences among targets (e.g., self-monitoring) and observers (e.g., OSN experience).

Full article on http://pss.sagepub.com

Adolescentes estão perdendo interesse nos blogs, indica pesquisa

BBC Brasil, 4 fev 2010

Os internautas mais jovens estão perdendo o interesse nos blogs e se voltando cada vez mais para formas mais curtas e portáteis de comunicação pela rede, enquanto a popularidade dos blogs entre os mais velhos se mantêm inalterada, segundo indica uma pesquisa americana.

Segundo o estudo feito pelo Pew Research Center, o número de jovens internautas americanos entre 12 e 17 anos que escrevem blogs caiu de 28% para 14% desde 2006.

Os adolescentes que disseram ter feito comentários em blogs de colegas caiu de 76% para 52% no mesmo período.

A pesquisa indica que os adolescentes vêm preferindo colocar postagens curtas em sites de redes sociais ou de micro-blogging, como o Facebook ou o Twitter, ou acessar a internet pelo telefone celular.

Ao mesmo tempo, a proporção de internautas adultos que disseram manter um blog na rede se manteve constante de 2006 para cá, em cerca de 10%.

Apesar de a popularidade dos blogs ter se mantido constante de uma maneira geral entre os adultos, a pesquisa mostra uma diferença clara quando analisados os dados entre os grupos por faixas etárias específicas.

A proporção de internautas entre 18 e 29 anos que mantêm um blog caiu de 24% para 15% desde 2007, enquanto entre os internautas com 30 anos ou mais essa proporção cresceu de 7% para 11%.

Redes sociais

Se por um lado a popularidade dos blogs tradicionais vem caindo entre os mais jovens, segundo indica a pesquisa, os dados mostram um envolvimento cada vez maior dos adolescentes em redes sociais na internet.

Segundo um estudante ouvido pela pesquisa, os adolescentes estão perdendo o interesse nos blogs porque eles precisam de rapidez e “as pessoas não acham a leitura tão divertida”.

De acordo com o estudo da Pew, 73% dos adolescentes americanos disseram usar sites de relacionamento social atualmente, contra 55% em novembro de 2006 e 65% em fevereiro de 2008.

A pesquisa indica ainda um aumento da popularidade do acesso à internet pelo celular – 55% dos jovens entre 18 e 29 anos e 27% dos adolescentes de 12 a 17 disseram acessar a rede dessa maneira.

Outro resultado interessante da pesquisa é que, ao contrário da maioria dos sites de relacionamento ou micro-blogging, o uso do Twitter é mais popular entre os adultos jovens do que entre os adolescentes.

Segundo o estudo, 8% dos adolescentes leem postagens ou escrevem no Twitter, contra 19% dos adultos maiores de 18 anos. Na faixa etária entre os 18 e os 29 anos, o uso da rede de micro-blogging chega a 37%.